A. B. Watson and A. J. Ahumada, “A model of how humans sense image motion,” Invest. Opthalmol. Vis. Sci. Suppl. 25, 14 (1984).
P. Thompson, “The coding of velocity of movement in the human visual system,” Vision Res. 24, 41–45 (1984).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
J. P. H. van Santen and G. Sperling, “Temporal covariance model of human motion perception,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1, 451–473 (1984).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
C. F. Stromeyer, R. E. Kronauer, J. C. Madsen, and S. A. Klein, “Opponent mechanisms in human vision,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1, 876–884 (1984).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
T. J. Long, “Why not compatible high-definition television?”IBA Tech. Rev. 21, 4–12 (1983); T. S. Robson, “Extended-definition television service,” Proc. IEE 129, 485–489 (1982).
E. H. Adelson and J. R. Bergen, “Spatio-temporal energy models for the Perception of Motion,”J. Opt. Soc. Am. 73, 1861 (1983).
E. H. Adelson, “Some new illusions, and some old ones, analyzed in terms of their Fourier components,” Invest. Opthalmol. Vis. Sci. Suppl. 22, 144 (1982).
P. Thompson, “Perceived rate of movement depends on contrast,” Vision Res. 22, 377–380 (1982).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
E. H. Adelson and J. A. Movshon “Phenomenal coherence of moving gratings,” Nature 200, 523–525 (1982).
[Crossref]
M. Fahle and T. Poggio, “Visual hyperacuity: spatio-temporal interpolation in human vision,” Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B 213, 451–477 (1981).
[Crossref]
A. B. Watson and J. G. Robson, “Discrimination at threshold: labelled detectors in human vision,” Vision Res. 21, 1115–1122 (1981).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
D. Marr and S. Ullman, “Direction selectivity and its use in early visual processing,” Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B 211, 151–180 (1981).
[Crossref]
P. Burt and G. Sperling, “Time, distance, and feature trade-offs in visual apparent motion,” Psych. Rev. 88, 171–195 (1981).
[Crossref]
S. M. Anstis, “The perception of apparent movement,” Phil. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. B 290, 153–168 (1980).
[Crossref]
A. B. Watson, P. G. Thompson, B. J. Murphy, and J. Nachmias, “Summation and discrimination of gratings moving in opposite directions,” Vision Res. 20, 341–347 (1980).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
D. H. Kelly, “Motion and vision, II. Stabilized spatio-temporal threshold surface,”J. Opt. Soc. Am. 69, 1340–349 (1979).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
M. J. Morgan, “Perception of continuity in stroboscopic motion: a temporal frequency analysis.” Vision Res 19, 491–500 (1979); “Analogue models of motion perception,” Phil. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. B 290, 117–135 (1980).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
H. R. Wilson and J. R. Bergen, “A four mechanism model for threshold spatial vision,” Vision Res. 19, 19–33 (1979).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
J. A. Movshon, I. D. Thompson, and D. J. Tolhurst, “Spatial summation in the receptive fields of simple cells in the cat’s striate cortex.”J. Physiol. (London) 283, 79–99 (1978).
J. A. Movshon, I. D. Thompson, and D. J. Tolhurst, “Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity of neurons in areas 17 and 18 of the cat’s visual contex,”J. Physiol. (London) 283, 101–120 (1978).
A. J. Pantle and L. Picciano, “A multi-stable movement display: Evidence for two separate motion systems in humans,” Science 193, 500–502 (1976).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
G. Sperling, “Movement perception in computer-driven visual displays,” Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. 8, 144–151 (1976).
[Crossref]
D. H. Tolhurst and J. A. Movshon, “Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity of striate cortical neurons,” Nature 257, 674–675 (1975).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
S. M. Anstis and B. J. Rogers, “Illusory reversal of visual depth and movement during changes of contrast,” Vision Res. 15, 957–961 (1975).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
D. J. Tolhurst, “Sustained and transient channels in human vision,” Vision Res. 15, 1151–1155 (1975).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
E. Levinson and R. Sekuler, “The independence of channels in human vision selective for direction of movement,”J. Physiol. London 250, 347–366 (1975).
[PubMed]
O. Braddick, “A short-range process in apparent motion,” Vision Res. 14, 519–529, (1974); “Low-level and high-level processes in apparent motion,” Phil. Trans. R. Soc. London B 290, 137–151 (1980).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
J. S. Lappin and H. H. Bell, “Perceptual differentiation of sequential visual patterns,” Percept. Psychophys. 12, 129–134 (1972).
[Crossref]
A. Pantle and R. Sekuler, “Contrast response of human visual mechanisms sensitive to orientation and motion.” Vision Res. 9, 397–406 (1969).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
F. W. Campbell and J. G. Robson, “Application of Fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings,”J. Physiol. (London) 197, 551–566 (1968).
D. H. Hubel and T. N. Wiesel, “Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat’s striate cortex,”J. Physiol. (London) 148, 574–591 (1959).
E. H. Adelson and J. R. Bergen, “Spatio-temporal energy models for the Perception of Motion,”J. Opt. Soc. Am. 73, 1861 (1983).
E. H. Adelson and J. A. Movshon “Phenomenal coherence of moving gratings,” Nature 200, 523–525 (1982).
[Crossref]
E. H. Adelson, “Some new illusions, and some old ones, analyzed in terms of their Fourier components,” Invest. Opthalmol. Vis. Sci. Suppl. 22, 144 (1982).
A. B. Watson, A. Ahumada, and J. E. Farrell, “The window of visibility: a psychophysical theory of fidelity in time-sampled visual motion displays,”NASA Tech. Paper TP-2211 (1983).
A. B. Watson and A. J. Ahumada, “A model of how humans sense image motion,” Invest. Opthalmol. Vis. Sci. Suppl. 25, 14 (1984).
A. B. Watson and A. J. Ahumada, “A look at motion in the frequency domain,”NASA Tech. Memo. TM-84352 (1983).
S. M. Anstis, “The perception of apparent movement,” Phil. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. B 290, 153–168 (1980).
[Crossref]
S. M. Anstis and B. J. Rogers, “Illusory reversal of visual depth and movement during changes of contrast,” Vision Res. 15, 957–961 (1975).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
S. M. Anstis, “Apparent Movement,” in Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Vol. VIII, Perception, R. Held, H. W. Leibowitz, and H.-L. Teuber, eds. (Springer-Verlag, New York, 1977).
S. M. Anstis, Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada (personal communication, 1981).
J. S. Lappin and H. H. Bell, “Perceptual differentiation of sequential visual patterns,” Percept. Psychophys. 12, 129–134 (1972).
[Crossref]
E. H. Adelson and J. R. Bergen, “Spatio-temporal energy models for the Perception of Motion,”J. Opt. Soc. Am. 73, 1861 (1983).
H. R. Wilson and J. R. Bergen, “A four mechanism model for threshold spatial vision,” Vision Res. 19, 19–33 (1979).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
J. R. Bergen and H. R. Wilson, “Prediction of flicker sensitivities from temporal three pulse data,” Vision Res. (to be published).
O. Braddick, “A short-range process in apparent motion,” Vision Res. 14, 519–529, (1974); “Low-level and high-level processes in apparent motion,” Phil. Trans. R. Soc. London B 290, 137–151 (1980).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
J. Hochberg and V. Brooks, “The perception of motion pictures,” in Handbook of Perception, E. C. Carterette and M. Friedmen, eds. (Academic, New York, 1978), Vol. 10.
J. Ross and D. Burr, “The psychophysics of motion,” in Proceedings of the Workshop of Vision, Brain, and Cooperative Computation, M. A. Arbib and A. R. Hanson eds. (U. Massachusetts Press, Amherst, Mass., 1983); Vision, Brain, and Cooperative Computation (Bradford, Amherst, Mass., to be published).
P. Burt and G. Sperling, “Time, distance, and feature trade-offs in visual apparent motion,” Psych. Rev. 88, 171–195 (1981).
[Crossref]
F. W. Campbell and J. G. Robson, “Application of Fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings,”J. Physiol. (London) 197, 551–566 (1968).
C. Enroth-Cugell and J. G. Robson, “The contrast sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells of the cat,”J. Physiol. London 187, 517–552 (1966).
[PubMed]
M. Fahle and T. Poggio, “Visual hyperacuity: spatio-temporal interpolation in human vision,” Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B 213, 451–477 (1981).
[Crossref]
A. B. Watson, A. Ahumada, and J. E. Farrell, “The window of visibility: a psychophysical theory of fidelity in time-sampled visual motion displays,”NASA Tech. Paper TP-2211 (1983).
J. Hochberg and V. Brooks, “The perception of motion pictures,” in Handbook of Perception, E. C. Carterette and M. Friedmen, eds. (Academic, New York, 1978), Vol. 10.
D. H. Hubel and T. N. Wiesel, “Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat’s striate cortex,”J. Physiol. (London) 148, 574–591 (1959).
J. S. Lappin and H. H. Bell, “Perceptual differentiation of sequential visual patterns,” Percept. Psychophys. 12, 129–134 (1972).
[Crossref]
E. Levinson and R. Sekuler, “The independence of channels in human vision selective for direction of movement,”J. Physiol. London 250, 347–366 (1975).
[PubMed]
T. J. Long, “Why not compatible high-definition television?”IBA Tech. Rev. 21, 4–12 (1983); T. S. Robson, “Extended-definition television service,” Proc. IEE 129, 485–489 (1982).
D. Marr and S. Ullman, “Direction selectivity and its use in early visual processing,” Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B 211, 151–180 (1981).
[Crossref]
M. J. Morgan, “Perception of continuity in stroboscopic motion: a temporal frequency analysis.” Vision Res 19, 491–500 (1979); “Analogue models of motion perception,” Phil. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. B 290, 117–135 (1980).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
E. H. Adelson and J. A. Movshon “Phenomenal coherence of moving gratings,” Nature 200, 523–525 (1982).
[Crossref]
J. A. Movshon, I. D. Thompson, and D. J. Tolhurst, “Spatial summation in the receptive fields of simple cells in the cat’s striate cortex.”J. Physiol. (London) 283, 79–99 (1978).
J. A. Movshon, I. D. Thompson, and D. J. Tolhurst, “Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity of neurons in areas 17 and 18 of the cat’s visual contex,”J. Physiol. (London) 283, 101–120 (1978).
D. H. Tolhurst and J. A. Movshon, “Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity of striate cortical neurons,” Nature 257, 674–675 (1975).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
A. B. Watson, P. G. Thompson, B. J. Murphy, and J. Nachmias, “Summation and discrimination of gratings moving in opposite directions,” Vision Res. 20, 341–347 (1980).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
A. B. Watson, P. G. Thompson, B. J. Murphy, and J. Nachmias, “Summation and discrimination of gratings moving in opposite directions,” Vision Res. 20, 341–347 (1980).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
A. Pantle and R. Sekuler, “Contrast response of human visual mechanisms sensitive to orientation and motion.” Vision Res. 9, 397–406 (1969).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
A. J. Pantle and L. Picciano, “A multi-stable movement display: Evidence for two separate motion systems in humans,” Science 193, 500–502 (1976).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
D. E. Pearson, Transmission and Display of Pictorial Information (Wiley, New York, 1975).
A. J. Pantle and L. Picciano, “A multi-stable movement display: Evidence for two separate motion systems in humans,” Science 193, 500–502 (1976).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
M. Fahle and T. Poggio, “Visual hyperacuity: spatio-temporal interpolation in human vision,” Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B 213, 451–477 (1981).
[Crossref]
W. Reichardt, “Autocorrelation, a principle for the evaluation of sensory information by the central nervous system,” in Sensory Communication, W. A. Rosenblith, ed. (Wiley, New York, 1961).
A. B. Watson and J. G. Robson, “Discrimination at threshold: labelled detectors in human vision,” Vision Res. 21, 1115–1122 (1981).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
F. W. Campbell and J. G. Robson, “Application of Fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings,”J. Physiol. (London) 197, 551–566 (1968).
C. Enroth-Cugell and J. G. Robson, “The contrast sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells of the cat,”J. Physiol. London 187, 517–552 (1966).
[PubMed]
J. G. Robson, “Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity functions of the visual system,”J. Opt. Soc. Am. 56, 1141–1142 (1966).
[Crossref]
S. M. Anstis and B. J. Rogers, “Illusory reversal of visual depth and movement during changes of contrast,” Vision Res. 15, 957–961 (1975).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
J. Ross and D. Burr, “The psychophysics of motion,” in Proceedings of the Workshop of Vision, Brain, and Cooperative Computation, M. A. Arbib and A. R. Hanson eds. (U. Massachusetts Press, Amherst, Mass., 1983); Vision, Brain, and Cooperative Computation (Bradford, Amherst, Mass., to be published).
E. Levinson and R. Sekuler, “The independence of channels in human vision selective for direction of movement,”J. Physiol. London 250, 347–366 (1975).
[PubMed]
A. Pantle and R. Sekuler, “Contrast response of human visual mechanisms sensitive to orientation and motion.” Vision Res. 9, 397–406 (1969).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
J. P. H. van Santen and G. Sperling, “Temporal covariance model of human motion perception,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1, 451–473 (1984).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
P. Burt and G. Sperling, “Time, distance, and feature trade-offs in visual apparent motion,” Psych. Rev. 88, 171–195 (1981).
[Crossref]
G. Sperling, “Movement perception in computer-driven visual displays,” Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. 8, 144–151 (1976).
[Crossref]
J. A. Movshon, I. D. Thompson, and D. J. Tolhurst, “Spatial summation in the receptive fields of simple cells in the cat’s striate cortex.”J. Physiol. (London) 283, 79–99 (1978).
J. A. Movshon, I. D. Thompson, and D. J. Tolhurst, “Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity of neurons in areas 17 and 18 of the cat’s visual contex,”J. Physiol. (London) 283, 101–120 (1978).
P. Thompson, “The coding of velocity of movement in the human visual system,” Vision Res. 24, 41–45 (1984).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
P. Thompson, “Perceived rate of movement depends on contrast,” Vision Res. 22, 377–380 (1982).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
A. B. Watson, P. G. Thompson, B. J. Murphy, and J. Nachmias, “Summation and discrimination of gratings moving in opposite directions,” Vision Res. 20, 341–347 (1980).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
D. H. Tolhurst and J. A. Movshon, “Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity of striate cortical neurons,” Nature 257, 674–675 (1975).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
J. A. Movshon, I. D. Thompson, and D. J. Tolhurst, “Spatial summation in the receptive fields of simple cells in the cat’s striate cortex.”J. Physiol. (London) 283, 79–99 (1978).
J. A. Movshon, I. D. Thompson, and D. J. Tolhurst, “Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity of neurons in areas 17 and 18 of the cat’s visual contex,”J. Physiol. (London) 283, 101–120 (1978).
D. J. Tolhurst, “Sustained and transient channels in human vision,” Vision Res. 15, 1151–1155 (1975).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
D. Marr and S. Ullman, “Direction selectivity and its use in early visual processing,” Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B 211, 151–180 (1981).
[Crossref]
S. Ullman, The Interpretation of Visual Motion (MIT U. Press, Cambridge, Mass., 1979).
A. B. Watson and A. J. Ahumada, “A model of how humans sense image motion,” Invest. Opthalmol. Vis. Sci. Suppl. 25, 14 (1984).
A. B. Watson and J. G. Robson, “Discrimination at threshold: labelled detectors in human vision,” Vision Res. 21, 1115–1122 (1981).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
A. B. Watson, P. G. Thompson, B. J. Murphy, and J. Nachmias, “Summation and discrimination of gratings moving in opposite directions,” Vision Res. 20, 341–347 (1980).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
A. B. Watson, A. Ahumada, and J. E. Farrell, “The window of visibility: a psychophysical theory of fidelity in time-sampled visual motion displays,”NASA Tech. Paper TP-2211 (1983).
A. B. Watson and A. J. Ahumada, “A look at motion in the frequency domain,”NASA Tech. Memo. TM-84352 (1983).
D. H. Hubel and T. N. Wiesel, “Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat’s striate cortex,”J. Physiol. (London) 148, 574–591 (1959).
H. R. Wilson and J. R. Bergen, “A four mechanism model for threshold spatial vision,” Vision Res. 19, 19–33 (1979).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
J. R. Bergen and H. R. Wilson, “Prediction of flicker sensitivities from temporal three pulse data,” Vision Res. (to be published).
G. Sperling, “Movement perception in computer-driven visual displays,” Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. 8, 144–151 (1976).
[Crossref]
T. J. Long, “Why not compatible high-definition television?”IBA Tech. Rev. 21, 4–12 (1983); T. S. Robson, “Extended-definition television service,” Proc. IEE 129, 485–489 (1982).
A. B. Watson and A. J. Ahumada, “A model of how humans sense image motion,” Invest. Opthalmol. Vis. Sci. Suppl. 25, 14 (1984).
E. H. Adelson, “Some new illusions, and some old ones, analyzed in terms of their Fourier components,” Invest. Opthalmol. Vis. Sci. Suppl. 22, 144 (1982).
E. H. Adelson and J. R. Bergen, “Spatio-temporal energy models for the Perception of Motion,”J. Opt. Soc. Am. 73, 1861 (1983).
D. H. Kelly, “Motion and vision, II. Stabilized spatio-temporal threshold surface,”J. Opt. Soc. Am. 69, 1340–349 (1979).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
J. G. Robson, “Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity functions of the visual system,”J. Opt. Soc. Am. 56, 1141–1142 (1966).
[Crossref]
J. A. Movshon, I. D. Thompson, and D. J. Tolhurst, “Spatial summation in the receptive fields of simple cells in the cat’s striate cortex.”J. Physiol. (London) 283, 79–99 (1978).
F. W. Campbell and J. G. Robson, “Application of Fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings,”J. Physiol. (London) 197, 551–566 (1968).
D. H. Hubel and T. N. Wiesel, “Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat’s striate cortex,”J. Physiol. (London) 148, 574–591 (1959).
J. A. Movshon, I. D. Thompson, and D. J. Tolhurst, “Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity of neurons in areas 17 and 18 of the cat’s visual contex,”J. Physiol. (London) 283, 101–120 (1978).
C. Enroth-Cugell and J. G. Robson, “The contrast sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells of the cat,”J. Physiol. London 187, 517–552 (1966).
[PubMed]
E. Levinson and R. Sekuler, “The independence of channels in human vision selective for direction of movement,”J. Physiol. London 250, 347–366 (1975).
[PubMed]
D. H. Tolhurst and J. A. Movshon, “Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity of striate cortical neurons,” Nature 257, 674–675 (1975).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
E. H. Adelson and J. A. Movshon “Phenomenal coherence of moving gratings,” Nature 200, 523–525 (1982).
[Crossref]
J. S. Lappin and H. H. Bell, “Perceptual differentiation of sequential visual patterns,” Percept. Psychophys. 12, 129–134 (1972).
[Crossref]
S. M. Anstis, “The perception of apparent movement,” Phil. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. B 290, 153–168 (1980).
[Crossref]
D. Marr and S. Ullman, “Direction selectivity and its use in early visual processing,” Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B 211, 151–180 (1981).
[Crossref]
M. Fahle and T. Poggio, “Visual hyperacuity: spatio-temporal interpolation in human vision,” Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B 213, 451–477 (1981).
[Crossref]
P. Burt and G. Sperling, “Time, distance, and feature trade-offs in visual apparent motion,” Psych. Rev. 88, 171–195 (1981).
[Crossref]
A. J. Pantle and L. Picciano, “A multi-stable movement display: Evidence for two separate motion systems in humans,” Science 193, 500–502 (1976).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
M. J. Morgan, “Perception of continuity in stroboscopic motion: a temporal frequency analysis.” Vision Res 19, 491–500 (1979); “Analogue models of motion perception,” Phil. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. B 290, 117–135 (1980).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
H. R. Wilson and J. R. Bergen, “A four mechanism model for threshold spatial vision,” Vision Res. 19, 19–33 (1979).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
O. Braddick, “A short-range process in apparent motion,” Vision Res. 14, 519–529, (1974); “Low-level and high-level processes in apparent motion,” Phil. Trans. R. Soc. London B 290, 137–151 (1980).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
A. Pantle and R. Sekuler, “Contrast response of human visual mechanisms sensitive to orientation and motion.” Vision Res. 9, 397–406 (1969).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
A. B. Watson, P. G. Thompson, B. J. Murphy, and J. Nachmias, “Summation and discrimination of gratings moving in opposite directions,” Vision Res. 20, 341–347 (1980).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
P. Thompson, “Perceived rate of movement depends on contrast,” Vision Res. 22, 377–380 (1982).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
S. M. Anstis and B. J. Rogers, “Illusory reversal of visual depth and movement during changes of contrast,” Vision Res. 15, 957–961 (1975).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
A. B. Watson and J. G. Robson, “Discrimination at threshold: labelled detectors in human vision,” Vision Res. 21, 1115–1122 (1981).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
P. Thompson, “The coding of velocity of movement in the human visual system,” Vision Res. 24, 41–45 (1984).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
D. J. Tolhurst, “Sustained and transient channels in human vision,” Vision Res. 15, 1151–1155 (1975).
[Crossref]
[PubMed]
S. M. Anstis, Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada (personal communication, 1981).
J. R. Bergen and H. R. Wilson, “Prediction of flicker sensitivities from temporal three pulse data,” Vision Res. (to be published).
S. Ullman, The Interpretation of Visual Motion (MIT U. Press, Cambridge, Mass., 1979).
S. M. Anstis, “Apparent Movement,” in Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Vol. VIII, Perception, R. Held, H. W. Leibowitz, and H.-L. Teuber, eds. (Springer-Verlag, New York, 1977).
J. Hochberg and V. Brooks, “The perception of motion pictures,” in Handbook of Perception, E. C. Carterette and M. Friedmen, eds. (Academic, New York, 1978), Vol. 10.
W. Reichardt, “Autocorrelation, a principle for the evaluation of sensory information by the central nervous system,” in Sensory Communication, W. A. Rosenblith, ed. (Wiley, New York, 1961).
A. B. Watson and A. J. Ahumada, “A look at motion in the frequency domain,”NASA Tech. Memo. TM-84352 (1983).
J. Ross and D. Burr, “The psychophysics of motion,” in Proceedings of the Workshop of Vision, Brain, and Cooperative Computation, M. A. Arbib and A. R. Hanson eds. (U. Massachusetts Press, Amherst, Mass., 1983); Vision, Brain, and Cooperative Computation (Bradford, Amherst, Mass., to be published).
D. E. Pearson, Transmission and Display of Pictorial Information (Wiley, New York, 1975).
A. B. Watson, A. Ahumada, and J. E. Farrell, “The window of visibility: a psychophysical theory of fidelity in time-sampled visual motion displays,”NASA Tech. Paper TP-2211 (1983).