Expand this Topic clickable element to expand a topic
Skip to content
Optica Publishing Group

Experimental investigation of the effect of binocular disparity on the visibility threshold of asymmetric noise in stereoscopic viewing

Open Access Open Access

Abstract

Stereoscopic images could have asymmetric distortions caused by image processing in capture, synthesis, and compression of them. In 3D perception in stereoscopic display, the visibility threshold of the asymmetric distortions in the left and right images is important, which is tolerable to the human visual system. In this paper, we investigate the effect of the binocular disparity on the visibility threshold of asymmetric noises in stereoscopic images via subjective assessments. Existing just-noticeable-difference (JND) models for stereoscopic images have not taken into account the effect of the disparity in stereoscopic viewing. In this paper, we subjectively assessed the visibility threshold of asymmetric noises in stereoscopic images according to the disparity. Subjective evaluations showed that large disparity magnitudes could make more tolerable to perceive the asymmetric noises in the stereoscopic viewing.

© 2016 Optical Society of America

1. Introduction

Stereoscopic three-dimensional (S3D) display systems provide viewers with a unique viewing experience by presenting binocular disparity into the left and right eyes. By fusing the binocular disparity, human visual system (HVS) perceives the relative depth of the presented scene; this is called as stereopsis [1]. The left and right images presented in S3D displays could have undesirable differences (e.g., different intensities and appearances in the left and right images) due to the asymmetric capture, synthesis, compression, and streaming of stereoscopic images. To properly address the perceptual issue of stereoscopic images, the visibility threshold of the asymmetric noises in the left and right images is important, which is tolerable to the HVS.

To determine the visibility threshold of the HVS, just-noticeable-difference (JND) models have been studied and extensively exploited in many image and video processing applications [2–6]. The visibility threshold could be affected by the features of stimulus such as the average background luminance behind pixels and the spatial non-uniformity of background luminance [6–10]. The conventional JND models for 2-D images would be not applicable for stereoscopic images because they did not consider the properties of stereoscopic viewing (e.g., binocular fusion of asymmetric noises). For S3D contents, Zhao et al. constructed a binocular JND (BJND) model [11]. They conducted psychophysical experiments to measure the visibility threshold of asymmetric noises according to the luminance and the contrast masking effects and binocular combinations of noise. However, their BJND model did not include the effect of disparity of visual stimuli on the visibility threshold.

In fact, many previous studies indicated that an amount of binocular disparity could have significant influences on the visual performance of human eyes [12,13]. Hoffman et al. showed that a conflict between accommodation and vergence coming from screen disparities (i.e., it is related to converging angles of the two eyes, and the distance to the screen [14].) degraded visual performance and increased binocular fusion time [12]. Zaroff et al. reported that the ability to perceive disparity difference (i.e., stereoacuity) could increase as the disparity decreased [13]. In an objective quality metric for synthesized stereoscopic images [15], the perceptual quality degradations induced by left and right mismatches could be accurately predicted by taking into account the disparity. Given the above previous studies, we speculate that the visibility threshold of asymmetric noise could be affected by the disparities of visual stimuli. To the best of our knowledge, there have been no attempts to investigate the relationship between binocular disparity and the visibility threshold of asymmetric noise.

The purpose of this paper is to investigate the effect of disparity magnitude (i.e., the amount of binocular disparity) on the visibility threshold of asymmetric noise. Subjective assessment has been conducted with stereoscopic square targets, which have a disparity varying from −1.00 to + 1.00 degree and a background luminance varying from 1 to 90 cd/m2. The square targets on the left and right images have asymmetric noises. Our subjective assessment experiment aims to measure the perceptible asymmetric noise amplitudes at varying disparities in stereoscopic viewing.

The experimental results showed that disparity magnitude could significantly affect the visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise in stereoscopic viewing. In particular, the threshold for detecting asymmetric noise increased as stimulus binocular disparity increased.

The remainder of this paper is organized as follows. In Section 2, we describe the experimental method used in our subjective measurement of the visibility threshold of asymmetric noise. Section 3 presents the experimental results and discussion. Finally, conclusions are drawn in Section 4.

2. Method

2.1 Subject

Fifteen subjects were participated in the experiment to measure the visibility threshold of asymmetric noise. The subject’s ages ranged from 22 to 31 with an average age of 25.53 years. All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal vision with a minimum stereoacuity of 50 arcsec (measured by the Randot stereotest®). The subjects were recruited under the approval of the KAIST institutional review board (IRB).

2.2 Apparatus

The subjective assessment was conducted in a dark room (room illumination was 9.34 lux as measured by Minolta T-10®) with a stereoscopic display (Redrover SDM-400®). The display consisted of a half mirror and two 40-inch liquid crystal displays (LCDs). The spatial resolution of each LCD was 1920 × 1080 pixels. The bit depth of the display was 8-bit. For the calibration of LCDs in the left and right displays, we calibrated the physical luminance values on the left and right displays from pixel intensity values with spectroradiometer (Minolta CS-1000®). Based on the definition of crosstalk [16,17], the crosstalk levels of the left and right displays were measured as 0.75% and 0.27%, which were lower than the visibility threshold of crosstalk [16]. The crosstalk levels were measured by the spectroradiometer (Minolta CS-1000®) as well. The viewing distance between a subject and the stereoscopic display was 1.5 meters, which was about three times the height of the display [18]. The horizontal and vertical viewing angles were 32.91 degrees and 18.85 degrees, respectively. The viewing environment was set up based on the recommendations of the ITU-R BT. 500-11 [18] and BT. 1438 [19].

2.3 Visual stimulus

As shown in Fig. 1(a), visual stimulus consisted of a square (5° × 5° of visual angle; corresponding to parafovea [20]), a fixation cross (0.8° × 0.8° of visual angle [21]), and a uniform background (chromaticity: D65, illumination: 28.87 cd/m2). The fixation cross had a zero disparity to signal the screen plane. As shown in Fig. 1(b), the 5° × 5° square wasdisplaced in a depth with a certain amount of disparity. Depth information of the square was generated by shifting squares to opposite direction in left and right views. Note the fixation cross always had a zero disparity. For asymmetric noise, the bipolar noise patterns with the independent amplitude, AL and AR were added into the central area (2° × 2° of visual angle; corresponding to the fovea [22]) in the left and right images, respectively [11]. The bipolar noise patterns ( + or -) were randomly generated. So, the noise patterns presented to the left and right eye were identical but independent amplitude. Table 1 shows the attributes of visual stimuli. As seen in Table 1, four different background luminance values (bg), four different noise amplitudes injected in the left image (AL), and nine different disparity values (d) were examined in this study. Note that the disparity values of visual stimuli were carefully selected to avoid visual discomfort effect in subjective assessment. Disparity values examined in this study were within the so-called comfort zone, which is generally considered to be about 1 degree of screen disparity [23]. As a result, a total number of 144 visual stimuli were generated for the subjective assessment (i.e., 144 = 4 background luminance values × 4 noise amplitude levels × 9 disparity values).

 figure: Fig. 1

Fig. 1 (a) Illustration of the visual stimulus of the left and right eye images. (b) Anaglyph view of the visual stimulus. Note that bg denotes the background luminance (cd/m2), d denotes the disparity (degree), AL and AR denote the noise amplitude (cd/m2) injected in the left and right images, respectively. The bipolar noise was randomly injected in the 2° × 2° central area (i.e., dashed region) in (a).

Download Full Size | PDF

Tables Icon

Table 1. Visual Stimulus Attributes

2.4 Procedure

A visual stimulus was randomly chosen from 144 visual stimuli and was presented to the subjects. Let AL denote the noise amplitude of the left image and AR(n) denote the noise amplitude in the right images assessed by the n-th subject. Given the noise amplitude in the left view (AL) at different disparities (d) and background luminance (bg), a subject (e.g., n-th subject) adjusted the noise amplitude in the right view (AR(n)) with the staircase method (reversal 3) [24] for detecting the just noticeable noises pair {AL, AR(n)}. Note that the noise amplitude in the right view started from the same amplitude in the left view. The noise amplitude in the right view indicates the visibility threshold which can evoke perceptible asymmetric noise in the stereoscopic viewing for the n-th subject. From all 15 subjects, we gathered the perceivable asymmetric noise pairs {AL, AR(n)} under 9 disparities and 4 background luminance levels. Then, the measured right noise amplitude AR (i.e., visibility threshold) that makes asymmetric noise perceptible was obtained by averaging individual thresholds (AR(n)). Note that for each trial, the subjects were instructed to hold their visual fixation on the fixation cross. In addition, the subjects were also instructed to maintain the binocular fusion state when the central square was perceived as a single vision (not double vision) [25].

The exposure time of a visual stimulus was not limited to provide sufficient time to judge the visibility threshold of asymmetric noise [26]. To avoid visual fatigue which could be induced in the assessment process for all visual stimuli, the test was paused after every 30 minutes and the subjects were instructed to relax their eyes [27]. Even within 30 minutes of testing, the test was stopped immediately when the subjects sensed any visual fatigue.

3. Results and discussions

Figure 2 shows the measured noise amplitudes in the right image which evoked perceptible difference (i.e., visibility threshold of asymmetric noise) in different disparities. In the Fig. 2, different markers represent different noise amplitudes injected in the left image (AL). As seen in the Fig. 2, the visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise increased as disparity magnitude (i.e., absolute disparity values) increased for all background luminance. For instance, for a case of no noise in the left image (AL = 0), the visibility threshold increased from 0.060 to 0.092 cd/m2 for 1cd/m2 of background luminance, from 0.042 to 0.067 cd/m2 for 9cd/m2 of background luminance, from 0.069 to 0.092 cd/m2 for 38cd/m2 of background luminance, and from 0.083 to 0.125 cd/m2 for 90cd/m2 of background luminance as the disparity magnitude increased from 0 to 1 degree, respectively. In addition, the visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise decreased as the noise amplitude injected in left image increased. For the large noise amplitude (AL = 0.05) in the left image, subjects perceived asymmetric noise in most of the disparity magnitudes. As seen in Figs. 2(a)-2(d), the visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise varied at different background luminance values [11].

 figure: Fig. 2

Fig. 2 Measured noise amplitudes in the right image that evoked perceptible difference (i.e., visibility threshold of asymmetric noise) along the disparity. (a) Visibility threshold for 1cd/m2 of background luminance, (b) for 9cd/m2 of background luminance, (c) for 38cd/m2 of background luminance, and (d) for 90cd/m2 of background luminance. The abscissa represents the disparity (in degree). The ordinate represents the measured visibility threshold (AR). In figures, different markers represent the different noise amplitudes injected in the left image (AL). The error bars represent the standard error of the mean. Note that subjects were instructed to hold their fixation on the fixation cross with a zero disparity in order to induce viewers’ vergence on the screen plane.

Download Full Size | PDF

To check the statistical significance of the effect of the disparity on the visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA test [28] was conducted. In the statistical analysis, the disparity (d) and the noise amplitude injected in the left image (AL) were considered as within-subjects variables. In general, for a within-subjects design, within-subjects variables were manipulated by testing each subject at each level of the variables. Table 2 shows results of the statistical significance test. As seen in the table, the effect of the disparity and the noise amplitude injected in the left image on the visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise was significant for each background luminance of a visual stimulus (p<0.001). In addition, the interaction between the disparity and the noise amplitude injected in the left image was significant at 95% confidence level (p<0.005). This result indicates that the effect of the disparity on the visibility threshold of an asymmetric noise varies with the noise amplitude injected in the left image.

Tables Icon

Table 2. Statistical Analysis Results Using the Two-way Repeated-measures ANOVA

In sum, the observations from the above subjective assessment results are as follows: 1) the visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise increased as the disparity magnitude increased for each background luminance value, and 2) the visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise decreased as the noise amplitude injected in the left image increased. Based on these observations in this study and the previous studies on binocular vision [11,29,30], a visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise for a background luminance, which includes both effects of the disparity and the noise amplitude in one eye image, can be modeled as

Thrbg(d,AL)=Alimit,bg(d)(1(A1Alimit,bg(d))λ)1λ,for1d1,and0A10.050.
where A1 denotes the noise amplitude in one eye image (e.g. A1 is AL in Fig. 2), and Thrbg denotes the visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise (the minimum noise amplitude in the other eye image that evokes the perceptible difference for a given background luminance). λ is a regression parameter, which was obtained by minimizing the sum of square errors between the model-calculated visibility thresholds and the subjectively measured visibility thresholds. Alimit,bg is the upper limit of Thrbg (i.e., A1 = 0) for given background luminance.

Based on the observation that subjectively measured visibility thresholds increased along the disparity magnitude in a convex form, we define Alimit,bg(d) as a quadratic polynomial function of d, which can be written as

Alimit,bg(d)=αd2+βd+γ.
where α, β, and γ are regression parameters. Table 3 summarizes the estimated parameter values and the goodness-of-fit statistics of the visibility threshold models.

Tables Icon

Table 3. Estimated Parameter Values and the Goodness-of-fit Statistics of Visibility Threshold Models

Figure 3 represents the modeled visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise along the disparity change for background luminance. The abscissa represents the disparity (in degree) and the ordinate represents the visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise. As shown in Fig. 3, the model captures the disparity effect on the visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise.

 figure: Fig. 3

Fig. 3 Modeled visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise along the disparity change. Each value in the dashed-line contour represents given noise amplitude in one eye image (A1). (a) Modeled visibility threshold for 1cd/m2 of background luminance, (b) for 9cd/m2 of background luminance, (c) for 38cd/m2 of background luminance, and (d) for 90cd/m2 of background luminance. Note that the colorbar is in log scale for display.

Download Full Size | PDF

We examined whether the modeled visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise is also valid for other subjects. To this end, an additional subjective assessment was conducted with newly recruited ten subjects [31,32]. To obtain the visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise for the new subjects, we used the same experimental setup, visual stimuli, and procedures as described in Section 2. Then, the visibility thresholds of the asymmetric noise were obtained for the new subjects, which were compared in the models in Fig. 3. The mean absolute difference (MAD) between the predicted visibility thresholds of the asymmetric noise and the measured visibility thresholds was as small as 0.0022 ± 0.0012 (mean ± std). In addition, the difference between the predicted visibility thresholds of the asymmetric noise and the measured visibility thresholds was not statistically significant (pairwise t-test [28]; t(143) = 1.582, p = 0.141). These results suggest that the modeled visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise could be valid for other subjects.

There are various potential sources of binocular mismatches [33]. Many stereoscopic image processing related to the binocular mismatch (e.g., objective quality assessment [15,34]) could achieve better performances by considering the BJND model, which took into account purely noise amplitude deviation [11]. In [34], the BJND was used as the weight of the quality score for binocular mismatched regions. A high BJND value at a pixel indicated that the pixel was less important. It could improve the performances of the quality assessment for stereo images including various noises (e.g., JPEG, Gaussian blur, white noise, etc.). In [15], the BJND model was used to detect the most important errors between the left and right images (LR critical areas). By determining the threshold based on the model, more accurate LR critical areas could be detected for quality assessment of the synthesized stereo images.

The above previous works indicate that noise amplitude deviation is one of the important factors affecting the binocular asymmetry in the stereoscopic viewing.

The study in this paper is in line with the previous works in terms of the use of BJND model. We can observe the impact of the disparity on the noise amplitude deviations. In addition, we can predict the visibility threshold model considering the effect of binocular disparity on the visibility threshold of asymmetric noises. It is challenging to generalize the effect of binocular disparity on various factors of binocular mismatches. The effect of binocular disparity on the visibility threshold of other binocular mismatches could be good research subject to be investigated as a further work.

4. Conclusion

In this paper, the effect of binocular disparity on the visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise was investigated. We subjectively assessed the visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise with different disparity values under the various noise levels and background luminance conditions. Experimental results showed that the binocular disparity could affect the visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise in the stereoscopic viewing. From the subjective assessment results shown in Fig. 2, we observed that the visibility threshold to notice the asymmetric noise increased as the disparity increased. The effect of the disparity on the visibility threshold was statistically significant as seen in Table 2. Based on the experiments, we devised the visibility threshold model containing the binocular disparity effect in the stereoscopic viewing. We believe that our observation and quantification of the visibility threshold can be utilized to provide more accurate estimation of stereoscopic perceptual effect. In the quality assessment application [34], the excessive distortions in mismatched regions can be assigned higher weights of quality score based on the visibility threshold, so that the quality of stereoscopic images could be better accessed.

References and links

1. I. P. Howard and B. J. Rogers, Seeing in Depth (I. Porteous, 2002).

2. Z. Lu, W. Lin, X. Yang, E. Ong, and S. Yao, “Modeling visual attention’s modulatory aftereffects on visual sensitivity and quality evaluation,” IEEE Trans. Image Process. 14(11), 1928–1942 (2005). [CrossRef]   [PubMed]  

3. R. Wolfgang, C. Podilchuk, and E. Delp, “Perceptual watermarks for digital images and video,” Proc. IEEE 87(7), 1108–1126 (1999). [CrossRef]  

4. L. J. Karam, N. G. Sadaka, R. Ferzli, and Z. A. Ivanovski, “An efficient selective perceptual-based super-resolution estimator,” IEEE Trans. Image Process. 20(12), 3470–3482 (2011). [CrossRef]   [PubMed]  

5. Z. Luo, L. Song, S. Zheng, and N. Ling, “H.264/AVC perceptual optimization coding based on JND-directed coefficient suppression,” IEEE Trans. Circ. Syst. Video Tech. 23(6), 935–948 (2013). [CrossRef]  

6. C. Chou and C. Chen, “A perceptually optimized 3-D subband codec for video communication over wireless channels,” IEEE Trans. Circ. Syst. Video Tech. 6(2), 143–156 (1996). [CrossRef]  

7. X. K. Yang, W. S. Ling, Z. K. Lu, E. P. Ong, and S. S. Yao, “Just noticeable distortion model and its applications in video coding,” Signal Process. Image Commun. 20(7), 662–680 (2005). [CrossRef]  

8. A. N. Netravali and B. G. Haslcell, Digital Pictures: Representation and Compression (Plenum, 1988).

9. R. Forchheimer and T. Kromamder, “Image coding-from waveforms in animation,” IEEE Trans. Acoust. Speech Signal Process. 37(12), 2008–2023 (1989). [CrossRef]  

10. N. S. Jayant, J. D. Johnston, and R. J. Safranek, “Signal compression based on models of human perception,” Proc. IEEE 81(10), 1385–1422 (1993). [CrossRef]  

11. Y. Zhao, Z. Chen, C. Zhu, Y.-P. Tan, and L. Yu, “Binocular just-noticeable-difference model for stereoscopic images,” IEEE Signal Process. Lett. 18(1), 19–22 (2011). [CrossRef]  

12. D. M. Hoffman, A. R. Girshick, K. Akeley, and M. S. Banks, “Vergence-accommodation conflicts hinder visual performance and cause visual fatigue,” J. Vis. 8(3), 33 (2008). [CrossRef]   [PubMed]  

13. C. M. Zaroff, M. Knutelska, and T. E. Frumkes, “Variation in stereoacuity: normative description, fixation disparity, and the roles of aging and gender,” Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 44(2), 891–900 (2003). [CrossRef]   [PubMed]  

14. N. Holliman, 3D Display Systems (IOP Press, 2004).

15. Y. J. Jung, H. G. Kim, and Y. M. Ro, “Critical binocular asymmetry measure for perceptual quality assessment of synthesized stereo 3D images in view synthesis,” IEEE Trans. Circ. Syst. Video Tech. 26(7), 1201–1214 (2016). [CrossRef]  

16. A. J. Woods, “Crosstalk in stereoscopic displays: a review,” J. Electron. Imaging 21(4), 040902 (2012). [CrossRef]  

17. J.-C. Liou, K. Lee, and J.-F. Huang, “Low crosstalk multi-view tracking 3-D display of synchro-signal LED scanning backlight system,” J. Disp. Technol. 7(8), 411–419 (2011). [CrossRef]  

18. ITU-R BT.500–11, “Methodology for the subjective assessment of the quality of television pictures,” (2002).

19. ITU-R BT.1438, “Subjective assessment for stereoscopic television pictures,” (2000).

20. J. Battista, M. Kalloniatis, and A. Metha, “Visual function: the problem with eccentricity,” Clin. Exp. Optom. 88(5), 313–321 (2005). [CrossRef]   [PubMed]  

21. F. Farzin and A. M. Norcia, “Impaired visual decision-making in individuals with amblyopia,” J. Vis. 11(14), 6 (2011). [CrossRef]   [PubMed]  

22. H. Sohn, Y. J. Jung, S. Lee, H. W. Park, and Y. M. Ro, “Attention model-based visual comfort assessment for stereoscopic depth perception,” in Proc. Int. Conf. Digital Signal Processing (IEEE, 2011), pp. 1–6. [CrossRef]  

23. M. Lambooij, W. Ijsselsteijn, M. Fortuin, and I. Heynderickx, “Visual discomfort and visual fatigue of stereoscopic displays: a review,” J. Imaging Sci. Technol. 53(3), 030201 (2009). [CrossRef]  

24. S. H. Schwartz, Visual Perception: A Clinical Orientation, (McGraw-Hill Medical, 2009), Chap. 11.

25. I. Tsirlin, L. M. Wilcox, and R. S. Allison, “The effect of crosstalk on the perceived depth from disparity and monocular occlusions,” IEEE Trans. Broadcast 57(2), 445–453 (2011). [CrossRef]  

26. D. Jokisch, I. Daum, and N. F. Troje, “Self recognition versus recognition of others by biological motion: viewpoint-dependent effects,” Perception 35(7), 911–920 (2006). [CrossRef]   [PubMed]  

27. Y. J. Jung, H. Sohn, S. I. Lee, Y. M. Ro, and H. W. Park, “Quantitative measurement of binocular color fusion limit for non-spectral colors,” Opt. Express 19(8), 7325–7338 (2011). [CrossRef]   [PubMed]  

28. G. Van Belle, Statistical rules of thumb (John Wiley and Sons, 2002).

29. W. J. Levelt, “Binocular brightness averaging and contour information,” Br. J. Psychol. 56(1), 1–13 (1965). [CrossRef]   [PubMed]  

30. A. I. Cogan, “Monocular sensitivity during binocular viewing,” Vision Res. 22(1), 1–16 (1982). [CrossRef]   [PubMed]  

31. L. Ma, K. Ngan, F. Zhang, and S. Li, “Adaptive block-size transform based just-noticeable difference model for images/videos,” Signal Process. Image Commun. 26(3), 162–174 (2011). [CrossRef]  

32. X. Zhang, W. Lin, and P. Xue, “Just-noticeable difference estimation with pixels in images,” J. Vis. Commun. Image Represent. 19(1), 30–41 (2008). [CrossRef]  

33. W. J. Tam, F. Speranza, S. Yano, K. Shimono, and H. Ono, “Stereoscopic 3D-TV: Visual comfort,” IEEE Trans. Broadcast 57(2), 335–346 (2011). [CrossRef]  

34. F. Shao, W. Lin, S. Gu, G. Jiang, and T. Srikanthan, “Perceptual full-reference quality assessment of stereoscopic images by considering binocular visual characteristics,” IEEE Trans. Image Process. 22(5), 1940–1953 (2013). [CrossRef]   [PubMed]  

Cited By

Optica participates in Crossref's Cited-By Linking service. Citing articles from Optica Publishing Group journals and other participating publishers are listed here.

Alert me when this article is cited.


Figures (3)

Fig. 1
Fig. 1 (a) Illustration of the visual stimulus of the left and right eye images. (b) Anaglyph view of the visual stimulus. Note that bg denotes the background luminance (cd/m2), d denotes the disparity (degree), AL and AR denote the noise amplitude (cd/m2) injected in the left and right images, respectively. The bipolar noise was randomly injected in the 2° × 2° central area (i.e., dashed region) in (a).
Fig. 2
Fig. 2 Measured noise amplitudes in the right image that evoked perceptible difference (i.e., visibility threshold of asymmetric noise) along the disparity. (a) Visibility threshold for 1cd/m2 of background luminance, (b) for 9cd/m2 of background luminance, (c) for 38cd/m2 of background luminance, and (d) for 90cd/m2 of background luminance. The abscissa represents the disparity (in degree). The ordinate represents the measured visibility threshold (AR). In figures, different markers represent the different noise amplitudes injected in the left image (AL). The error bars represent the standard error of the mean. Note that subjects were instructed to hold their fixation on the fixation cross with a zero disparity in order to induce viewers’ vergence on the screen plane.
Fig. 3
Fig. 3 Modeled visibility threshold of the asymmetric noise along the disparity change. Each value in the dashed-line contour represents given noise amplitude in one eye image (A1). (a) Modeled visibility threshold for 1cd/m2 of background luminance, (b) for 9cd/m2 of background luminance, (c) for 38cd/m2 of background luminance, and (d) for 90cd/m2 of background luminance. Note that the colorbar is in log scale for display.

Tables (3)

Tables Icon

Table 1 Visual Stimulus Attributes

Tables Icon

Table 2 Statistical Analysis Results Using the Two-way Repeated-measures ANOVA

Tables Icon

Table 3 Estimated Parameter Values and the Goodness-of-fit Statistics of Visibility Threshold Models

Equations (2)

Equations on this page are rendered with MathJax. Learn more.

Th r bg (d, A L )= A limit,bg (d) ( 1 ( A 1 A limit,bg (d) ) λ ) 1 λ ,for 1d1, and 0 A 1 0.050.
A limit,bg (d)=α d 2 +βd+γ.
Select as filters


Select Topics Cancel
© Copyright 2024 | Optica Publishing Group. All rights reserved, including rights for text and data mining and training of artificial technologies or similar technologies.